Why humans started using symbols

A series of innovative experiments at COBE Lab sheds light on the evolution of early symbolic behavior in humans ca. 100,000 years ago. 

 

Some of the earliest evidence of humans using symbols stems from about 100,000 years ago in Africa.

A central site of interest is the Blombos Cave, where patterns engraved in ochre pieces have been found. Another is the Diepkloof Rock Shelter, where engraved ostrich eggshells have been found.

The engravings at both sites were made over a period of more than 30,000 years.

The first engravings were simple configurations of parallel lines, but over time they grew more complex, with greater symmetry and crossing lines.

A group of researchers headed by Kristian Tylén wanted to understand why the engravings evolved over time in the way they did

They explored this through a series of experiments at COBE.

Experiments

1) In one experiment, participant had vivid flickering colors presented to their dominant eye and outlines of the engraved patterns presented to their other eye. This meant that the more dominant visual of flickering colors would initially override their perception of the engravings.

The researchers found that the later engravings entered participants’ conscious perception faster this way than earlier engravings. This suggests that the engravings evolved over time to be more salient, standing out to our visual perceptual system.

2) Another experiment had participants reproduce engravings from memory. They were more accurate in reproducing later rather than earlier engravings.

This suggests that the engravings evolved over time to be easier to remember and reproduce.

3) In a third experiment, participants rated later engravings more likely to have been made by a human than earlier engravings.

This suggests that the engravings evolved over time to look more intentional.

4) A fourth experiment found that later engravings were easier to recognize as coming from the same or different caves than earlier engravings.

This suggests that the engravings evolved distinct elements of style over time, potentially signifying group identity.

5) In a fifth experiment, participants did not find it easier to distinguish later engravings from each other on an individual basis than earlier engravings.

The fact that the engravings did not evolve to be more easily distinguishable from each other suggests that they did not function as fully developed referential, denotational symbols—that is, they did not function like words or signs that have specific meanings and refer to specific things.

Conclusions

The results of the experiments suggest that these early examples of symbols did not function like words or signs with specific meanings. Instead, they may have served as aesthetic decorations and as socially transmitted traditions marking the sociocultural identity of the people who made them. This would explain why they evolved over time to be more salient, memorable, and expressive of human intent, and to reflect a particular site-specific style.

There is thus reason to believe that these were the original purposes that first drove humans to use symbols some 100,000 years ago.

The study was called "The Evolution of Early Symbolic Behavior in Homo Sapiens" and was published in the journal PNAS in 2020.

The short story

  • Some of the earliest evidence of humans using symbols are 100,000 year old engraved patterns in pieces of ochre and ostrich eggshells. The engravings were made over a period of more than 30,000 years.
  • The researchers found evidence that the patterns evolved over time to be more salient, to be easier to remember, to look more intentional, and to express an identifiable style. However, they did not evolve to be easier to distinguish from each other on an individual basis.
  • This suggests that they did not serve as words or signs with specific meanings but that they were rather aesthetic decorations, socially transmitted traditions, and markers of identity.

The researchers

Kristian Tylén

Professor at the Department of Linguistics, Cognitive Science and Semiotics, Aarhus University

Other researchers:

  • Riccardo Fusaroli (Department of Linguistics, Cognitive Science and Semiotics, Aarhus University)
  • Sergio Rojo (Department of Linguistics, Cognitive Science and Semiotics, Aarhus University)
  • Katrin Heimann (Interacting Minds Centre, Aarhus University)
  • Nicolas Fay (School of Psychological Science, University of Western Australia)
  • Niels N. Johannsen (Department of Archeology and Heritage Studies, Aarhus Universitet)
  • Felix Riede (Department of Archeology and Heritage Studies, Aarhus University)
  • Marlize Lombard (Palaeo-Research Institute, University of Johannesburg)